Are We Seeing What We Want See? The Human Factor

I recently visited several pet stores to personally witness interactions with reptiles. One bearded dragon’s behavior held my attention for a significant amount of time. Perched closest to the basking light it shared with 20-30 other juvenile bearded dragons, it ran to the front of the enclosure and stared at me intensely. After a while of camping out at the front, it ran back to the same basking spot, pushing the competition out of the way. Warmed up, it ran back to the front of the enclosure.
My initial thought was that the beardie was exhibiting territorial behavior, acting as an alpha among the juveniles. I asked to hold it and expected heavy breathing, petrified behavior, or quick evasive maneuvers. To my surprise, it cocked its head to the side and continued to look at me curiously. I held my hand flat, and it didn't run.
I have never seen a reptile exhibit such behavior; it reminded me more of a dog than a reptile. It didn't seem to associate me with food or see me as a source of safety. As the alpha in the exhibit, it wasn't trying to escape, and it wasn't aggressive, so I don't think it was protecting the others. This interaction didn't feel like a gamble but more of an opportunity to observe a truly unique bearded dragon. The behavior appeared to be intentional and not just an act of survival. But was it? This interaction raises the question: where does the line between survival instinct and genuine cognitive ability lie?
Thermoregulation: The Simplest Explanation
The most straightforward explanation for this bearded dragon's behavior lies in basic thermoregulatory needs rather than complex thinking. As (Roth et al., 2019) emphasize, a reptile's ectothermic physiology creates fundamental differences in its behavior patterns. Unlike mammals, reptiles depend entirely on external heat sources to maintain the optimal body temperature for all their biological processes.
The pattern observed—running to the basking spot, competing for the best position, then returning to the front—fits perfectly within thermoregulatory behavior. Reptilian cognitive performance fluctuates dramatically with body temperature, meaning that what appears as "intentional" behavior might simply reflect how active the dragon is at a certain temperature (Roth et al., 2019). When adequately warmed, the dragon becomes more active and exploratory. As its body temperature drops, it returns to the heat source.
The act of "pushing competition out of the way" is a classic example of basking spot competition, a well-documented survival strategy. In crowded captive conditions, access to heat becomes a limited resource. The dragon’s apparent dominance simply reflects its successful competition for thermal resources rather than a complex social behavior.
However, this explanation has its limits. The dragon's calm response to handling and lack of typical stress behaviors (like heavy breathing, freezing, or attempting to escape) suggests something more than simple thermoregulation was happening. Most reptiles show predictable stress responses when removed from optimal thermal environments, yet this individual remained curious and engaged (Hamilton et al., 2022).
Beyond Instinct: Social Recognition and Environmental Assessment
Recent research reveals that reptiles possess more sophisticated social and cognitive abilities than once believed. Reptiles can demonstrate individual recognition, assess their environment, and adjust their behavior based on the situation (Siviter et al., 2017). The bearded dragon's intense stare and head cocking might have been genuine social interest rather than just a move for a better basking spot.
Studies show that the incubation environment where a lizard is raised can affect its social cognition as an adult, suggesting that early life factors influence later social behavior (Siviter et al., 2017). Dragons raised in complex social environments may be better at assessing and interacting with new social stimuli, including humans.
"Gaze following" research provides a particularly relevant context. Some reptile species have sophisticated visual attention and gaze-following abilities that were previously thought to be limited to highly social mammals (Roth et al., 2019). The dragon's sustained eye contact and head movements could represent genuine social assessment.
Still, a healthy dose of skepticism is warranted. Pet store environments create artificial social contexts that can promote unusual behaviors. Constant exposure to humans could condition dragons to associate our presence with feeding, cleaning, or other environmental changes. This could create learned behaviors that mimic curiosity but are actually just for survival.
The Stress Response Paradox
Research on behavior under artificial conditions reveals complex patterns in how reptiles react to stress. As (Hamilton et al., 2022) found, a reptile’s behavioral diversity and use of space change significantly in response to environmental modifications, including human presence. Some species become more investigative when visitor patterns change, while others withdraw and hide.
The bearded dragon's calm demeanor during handling goes against typical stress response patterns, but it might actually be an adaptation to chronic stress rather than genuine comfort. Repeated handling in a pet store could lead to learned helplessness or a habituation to stress instead of a positive social bond.
Alternatively, the dragon's behavior might be neophilia—an attraction to new stimuli—which serves important survival functions in the wild. Investigating new elements in the environment helps animals assess potential threats, resources, or territorial changes. What looked like curiosity toward humans could simply be an adaptive neophilic response.
The research also shows that reptiles can have measurable physiological stress responses even when their behavior suggests they are calm (Hamilton et al., 2022). Heart rate monitoring in handled reptiles reveals elevated stress markers despite their outwardly calm appearance, which complicates how we interpret their behavior.
Individual Personality and Variation
Emerging research identifies substantial individual variation in reptile behavior, even within the same species (Crisante et al., 2023). Some individuals are consistently bold and exploratory, while others remain consistently cautious. This personality research suggests that the bearded dragon I saw might be on the extreme end of the boldness-shyness spectrum rather than a typical example of its species.
Behavioral syndromes in reptiles correlate with physiological and neurological differences between individuals (Roth et al., 2019). Bold individuals may have different stress thresholds, social tolerance levels, or sensory processing patterns that influence their responses to new stimuli.
The "fancy" label on the enclosure brings up additional questions. Captive breeding for certain physical traits might unintentionally select for behavioral characteristics. If breeders consistently choose dragons that tolerate handling well, those behavioral traits could become linked to specific physical features through artificial selection.
The genetic uncertainty of these "bargain bin" dragons makes interpretation even harder. Unknown lineages might carry behavioral variations that seem unusual but are actually normal for that broader gene pool. Without a breeding history, it's hard to tell if what you see is an individual's personality or a genetic abnormality.
The Survival-Cognition Continuum
The fundamental question—whether the observed behaviors reflect survival tactics or genuine cognition—may be a false choice. Modern cognitive research recognizes that survival-relevant behaviors can involve sophisticated information processing without requiring human-like consciousness or intentionality (Roth et al., 2019).
Consider a dog, for example. A dog cries to be walked or nudges your hand for attention. While these actions seem to signal "wants," they're learned because they fulfill "needs"—the need for exercise, for food, or for affection. Canines can be trained to hunt with their noses or detect diseases by learning different indicators, but these abilities are often reinforced by rewards that serve a survival need. This is the very essence of the fine line between intelligence and cognitive behavior: Is it measured by how much an animal can be taught, even if that learning is motivated by basic needs rather than a complex desire?
Reptiles demonstrate complex spatial learning, long-term memory formation, and environmental assessment abilities that clearly serve survival functions while involving genuine cognitive processes. The bearded dragon's behavior could simultaneously represent an adaptive survival response and legitimate cognitive engagement.
The challenge isn't to determine if a behavior is "cognitive" or "survival-based" but to understand how different levels of information processing contribute to behavioral flexibility and environmental adaptation. Simple reflexes and complex cognition represent the ends of a continuum, not two separate categories.
Recognizing this continuum allows for a more nuanced interpretation of reptile behavior, without either dismissing their apparent sophistication as mere anthropomorphism or over interpreting simple responses as evidence of human-like intelligence.
The bearded dragon's behavior remains so intriguing precisely because it challenges simple categorization. Whether it represents thermoregulatory optimization, social assessment, stress adaptation, individual personality, or genuine curiosity, the interaction shows the complexity that underlies what we might perceive as simple reptile behavior. To truly understand these animals, we must move beyond simplistic distinctions and appreciate the sophisticated information processing that enables successful environmental adaptation across all species.